Trial participants’ self-reported understanding of randomisation phrases in participation information leaflets can be high, but acceptability of some descriptions is low, especially those linked to gambling and luck

Frances Shiely* (Corresponding Author), Ellen Murphy, Katie Gilles, Kerry Hood, Lydia O’Sullivan, Nicola Harman, Talia Isaacs, Shaun Treweek

*Corresponding author for this work

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

8 Downloads (Pure)

Abstract

Background
Evidence indicates that trial participants often struggle to understand participant information leaflets (PILs) for clinical trials, including the concept of randomisation. We analysed the language used to describe randomisation in PILs and determine the most understandable and acceptable description through public and participant feedback.

Methods
We collected 280 PILs/informed consent forms and one video animation from clinical research facilities/clinical trial units in Ireland and the UK. We extracted text on how randomisation was described, plus trial characteristics. We conducted content analysis to group the randomisation phrases inductively. We then excluded phrases that appeared more than once or were very similar to others. The final list of randomisation phrases was then presented to an online panel of participants and the public. Panel members were asked to rate each phrase on a 5-point Likert scale in terms of their understanding of the phrase, confidence in their understanding and acceptability of the phrase.

Results
Two hundred and eighty PILs and the transcribed text from one video animation represented 229 ongoing or concluded trials. The pragmatic content analysis generated five inductive categories: (1) explanation of why randomisation is required in trials; (2) synonyms for randomisation; (3) comparative randomisation phrases; (4) elaborative phrases for randomisation (5) and phrases that describe the process of randomisation. We had 48 unique phrases, which were shared with 73 participants and members of the public. Phrases that were well understood were not necessarily acceptable. Participants understood, but disliked, comparative phrases that referenced gambling, e.g. toss of a coin, like a lottery, roll of a die. They also disliked phrases that attributed decision-making to computers or automated systems. Participants liked plain language descriptions of what randomisation is and those that did not use comparative phrases.

Conclusions
Potential trial participants are clear on their likes and dislikes when it comes to describing randomisation in PILs. We make five recommendations for practice.
Original languageEnglish
Article number391
Number of pages12
JournalTrials
Volume25
Issue number1
Early online date18 Jun 2024
DOIs
Publication statusPublished - 18 Jun 2024

Bibliographical note

Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the clinical research facilities and clinical trial units who provided us with the PILs to complete this work

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. The PILs will be made available on the Trial Forge website (https://www.trialforge.org/) by year end 2024.

Funding

This research was funded by the Health Research Board—Trials Methodology Research Network (HRB-TMRN) in Ireland supported by grant HRB-TMRN-2017–001.

FundersFunder number
Health Research Board—Trials Methodology Research Network (HRB-TMRN)HRB-TMRN-2017–001

    Keywords

    • Randomisation
    • Trials methodology
    • randomised controlled trial (RCT)
    • Participant information leafets
    • Informed consent

    Fingerprint

    Dive into the research topics of 'Trial participants’ self-reported understanding of randomisation phrases in participation information leaflets can be high, but acceptability of some descriptions is low, especially those linked to gambling and luck'. Together they form a unique fingerprint.

    Cite this